
HOUSTON – Contrary to popular notions, people at the high end of the autism spectrum disorder continuum suffer most from an inability to model “self” rather than impaired ability to respond to others, said researchers from the Baylor College of Medicine and The University of Alabama in a report appearing in the journal Neuron.
This inability to model “self” can disrupt an individual’s ability to understand the world as a whole, said Dr. P. Read Montague Jr., professor of neuroscience, and director of the Human Neuroimaging Lab and the Computational Psychiatry Unit at BCM. “It’s an interesting disconnect.”
Using a functional magnetic resonance imaging, or fMRI, scanner, the researchers scanned the brains of people considered “high functioning” autistics because they have normal or high normal intelligence quotients but many of the symptoms of people with autism. During this procedure, the researchers identified a pattern of activity or “signature” in the brain that identified those with autism. The level of activity correlates with the severity of the autistic symptoms. The less activity there is, the more serious the symptoms. The finding could lead to a test to speed diagnosis.
“The idea of identifying a neural signal for autism is very exciting,” said Dr. Laura Klinger, associate professor of psychology at The University of Alabama and co-author of the study appearing in the current issue of the journal. “Currently, our gold standard approach to diagnosing autism is through parent interviews and behavioral observation.
“This kind of research has important implications for early diagnosis which is vital,” said Klinger, who founded and directs a UA research clinic specializing in autism and related disorders. “Children who receive early diagnosis and intervention show substantially higher IQ, better academic achievement, and stronger social skills.”
“We are very excited about the usefulness of the hyperscanning technology and economic games as new tools to probe autism. Our hope is that these same approaches can be used to probe a wide range of psychopathologies,” said Dr. Pearl Chiu, first author on the study and an assistant professor in the departments of neuroscience and psychiatry at BCM.
To understand the behavioral patterns of people with autism spectrum disorder, the researchers used a technique called hyperscanning, which enabled them to scan two brains simultaneously while the research subjects played a trust game. Hyperscanning was developed in Montague’s laboratory.
In the trust game, one player receives an amount of money and then sends whatever amount he or she wants to the other player via computer message. The amount sent is tripled and the player at the other end then decides how much of the tripled amount to send back. The game has several rounds.
During this interaction, the researchers evaluated the brains’ response by watching bright spots in the brain that represent increased blood flow and thus brain activity. Prior work had shown that during the trust game, most of the activity occurs in an area called the cingulate cortex.
“We’ve known for a long time that autism is a neurological brain disorder,” said Dr. Mark Klinger, associate professor of psychology at UA. “However, it has proven very difficult to find the specific regions of the brain that differ in autism. This research could focus a great deal of new investigation into the cingulate cortex as an area that may be crucial to the brain and thinking difference in persons with autism.”
To hone their picture of the “self” response in that part of the brain, the researchers had 81 athletes (football, baseball and soccer players as well as members of the Houston Ballet) take part in an imagining task. They watched clips of various athletic activities while in the scanner. They then imagined themselves performing those activities. The pattern of activity in the cingulate cortex during that “imagining” reflected the “self” response.
Later the scientists identified the same “self” response in the cingulate cortices of normal subjects when they decided how much money to send to the other person. The pattern contrasted with the “other” response seen when the actions of their partner in the trust game were revealed to them.
The researchers then brought in 18 adolescent males with high functioning autism to play the game. Montague and his colleagues determined that the subjects understood the game and helped them adjust to the scanning procedure.
“It was the first time an autistic kid had been scanned in a social exchange,” said Montague.
The adolescents did not play the game differently from their partners, who were taken from a population of similar teens who did not have autism. They made similar amounts of money overall and round by round.
However, when the researchers scanned the brains of the youngsters with autism during the trust game, they found that the youngsters’ “self” responses were dim compared to those of normal subjects. Not only that, but the more serious the subject’s autistic symptoms, the dimmer the response.
The response occurred in the cingulate cortex. In a normal “self” response there, the brightest area was in the middle of that area of the brain. That response was significantly less in the brains of the youngsters with autism.
“They did remarkable well when playing the game; they won about the same amount of money as a typical person without autism,” said Mark Klinger, co-author of the study. “However they showed very different patterns of brain activation which reflects the very different thought processes they were using while playing the game. It seems that the participants with autism were less likely to think about themselves and the impressions they create on others.”
“The genius of this study was to recognize the primary deficit with this spectrum of disorders,” said Dr. Michael Friedlander, chair of neuroscience at BCM. “Then they took that information dealing with the deficit social cognition and invented a new kind of experiment and technology to probe it.”
Others who took part in this study included M. Amin Kayali, Kenneth T. Kishida and Damon Tomlin of BCM.
Funding for this work came from The Kane Family Foundation,The Dana Foundation and Autism Speaks, the National Institute on Drug Abuse, the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke,The Angel Williamson Imaging Center, and the American Psychological Association.
The paper is available at www.neuron.org
The department of psychology at The University of Alabama is part of the College of Arts and Sciences, the University’s largest division and the largest liberal arts college in the state. Students from the College have won numerous national awards including Rhodes Scholarships, Goldwater Scholarships and memberships on the USA Today Academic All American Team.
Contact
Chris Bryant, UA Assistant Director of Media Relations, 205/348-8323, cbryant@ur.ua.edu
Graciela Gutierrez, Baylor College of Medicine, ggutierr@bcm.edu, 713/798-4710